[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]
Article: Understanding risk
I, and I am sure others on this list, have an interesting in understanding
and communicating risk, e.g., why are you so afraid of an airliner hitting a
nuclear reactor? I found the following article that appeared in today's
Washsington Post to be interesting. I like the author and have read several
of his books. Hey, I was originally going to be an engineering, but found
physics to be more fun
.
-- John
John Jacobus, MS
Certified Health Physicist
3050 Traymore Lane
Bowie, MD 20715-2024
jenday1@email.msn.com (H)
-----------------------
To view the entire article, go to
http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/articles/A64827-2002Jun29.html
We've Got More Risk Than Our Brains Can Handle
By Henry Petroski
Risk is ubiquitous. Each of us is born with a life expectancy, and like it
or not we play the odds every day. Numbers can be put on the risks we face
from cradle to grave, but it is not possible to play life strictly by the
numbers. That is why each of us, in responding to risk, makes personal
decisions based not simply on quantitative measures but on qualitative
interpretations as well. We party on arsenic-impregnated decks. We play golf
in thunderstorms. We build multistory houses despite the risk of falling
down stairs. These days -- in a climate of heightened awareness of risk,
both real and perceived -- many public policy decisions appear to be made in
much the same way. Decisions come ultimately from the gut rather than from
the computer.
It is telling that the Department of Homeland Security has introduced a
color scale -- a qualitative visual gauge -- instead of a more easily
remembered numerical scale for the level of alert. The kinds of risks that
terrorists present are virtually impossible to quantify. There is simply too
much that we do not know about their timetable, their tactics and their
technology. There is also very little historical precedent in our country
for the kinds of risks we now face.
Numbers will no doubt in time be attached to a host of new risks, but
interpreting those numbers and assigning them colors will require judgment
and hard decisions. The possibility of a smallpox attack, for example,
requires a decision about whether to vaccinate the entire population when
enough vaccine becomes available. Because vaccination itself carries risks,
the correct policy is not obvious. Recently, an advisory panel recommended
vaccinating critical health care and law enforcementpersonnel against
smallpox but, because of the risk of complications, not the general public.
Does the real risk of complications outweigh the potential risk associated
with a smallpox attack? This is not an easy question to answer. The risks of
vaccinating are limited but fairly certain; the cost of an attack could be
immense, but its likelihood is completely uncertain. How can they be
measured against each other?
The wildfires that continue to burn in several western states are clear
evidence that there are risks associated with living too close to nature.
Even if those risks could be quantified, the numbers will never tell us
exactly where the next fire will burn or how the wind will be blowing when
it does. It is doubtful that those who build and live in houses among the
trees base their decision on statistics. They choose to live where they do
because of the beauty and peacefulness that a sylvan setting affords. Public
policy on how to control forest fires -- or whether this should be done at
all -- is by all reports not a debate over numbers. It is a debate over
environmental philosophy.
Discussions over whether or not airline pilots should be allowed to carry
guns in the cockpit have also revolved more around perceptions of risk than
quantifiable measures of risk. This is not surprising, for how can the risks
associated with armed pilots be determined? There haven't been guns in
cockpits, at least officially, and so there are no solid statistics on what
happens when there are. There are no numbers on which to base a quantitative
argument. Rather, the debate has involved imagined scenarios of what could
happen with pistol-packing pilots in control. No doubt policymakers'
opinions on the issue can reflect their views on guns at least as much as
their concern for passenger safety.
Public debates about risk and other threatening concepts may be
accompanied by a lot of numbers and graphs and budgets, but in the final
analysis, decisions are often based on political and emotional grounds, if
not on confusing logic. A politician can seem to pay as much attention to
the numbers from opinion polls as to the numbers from scientific studies.
This happens especially when researchers themselves appear to disagree on
the fundamental scientific facts behind issues like global warming and its
risks. Scientists can also appear to have an odd perspective on risk.
According to the Washington Times, a document uncovered recently in the
Environmental Protection Agency's files argued that dumping toxic waste into
the Potomac was good for fish because it drove them away from fishing areas.
Scientists, like policymakers, are nothing if not human.
Qualitative considerations will always trump quantitative risk assessment
analyses in the public policy arena. Even when it may appear that policy
decisions are based strictly on the numbers, there is always some
overarching qualitative principle defining the choice. Numbers, and the
formulas that produce them, are not sufficient for decision making. This
fact has been clearly articulated by David Billington in his book "The
Innovators," which is about pioneers in engineering who put America on the
road to modernity.
Billington noted that engineering formulas are not mere expressions of
scientific fact. They also have profound social implications relating to
safety and economy. In the case of risk, the numbers alone do not say what
is acceptable or unacceptable in the public policy sector. That
determination involves a judgment based not on hard numbers but on soft
emotions.
The point is illustrated by considering the relative risks associated with
highway and air travel. Automobile accidents in the United States claim
about 40,000 lives a year, not to mention all the injuries suffered in
crashes. Commercial aircraft accidents in this country typically have
claimed no more than hundreds of lives a year, two orders of magnitude fewer
than highway accidents. The numbers partly reflect the fact that many more
people drive than fly. Still, the statistical risks are far higher on the
road. Yet many white-knuckled fliers prefer to drive their cars carelessly,
risking their own lives and the lives of others on the road. Among the
reasonsfor the irrational fear of flying relative to drivingis the sense of
individual control felt in a car, a clearly emotional response.
Emotions overwhelm numbers all over the map. It remains inexplicable that
less risky airplane travel is subject to strict safety procedures, which
pilots and flight attendants review before each takeoff, while children
riding on school buses are protected mainly by the yellow color of the
vehicle in which they ride.
Predictably, airplane travel is perceived to be even more risky since
Sept. 11. But on that day terrorists lost their main weapon, which was
surprise. Now, enhanced airport security combined with more secure cockpit
doors -- and most likely other measures that have not been publicly
revealed -- can be assumed to have made air travel less risky. The next
terrorist attack will likely not take place on an airplane, though we
second-guess terrorists at our peril, and so continued vigilance at airports
and in flight remains absolutely necessary. Letting down our guard is to
raise the level of risk.
It is human nature to react the way we have to the terrorist attacks. We
all alter our perception of risk after something that was thought to be a
remote possibility becomes a clear reality.The 1993 truck-bomb attack on the
World Trade Center, though relatively unsuccessful, revealed the true risks
involved in allowing unfettered parking beneath the tallest building in New
York. Closing the parking garage to the public and instituting tight
security procedures reduced the risk of another bomb being carried into the
building, but attack by a fuel-laden airplane was not a very bright blip on
the risk radar screen. In retrospect, of course, the building was at risk
from air attack all along.
Being human, public policymakers are as susceptible as laypersons to
minimizing the perception of risks that we all wish did not exist. However,
remote risks realized, as they were last September, provide a rude awakening
to us all. Everyone can now imagine risks where they were not acknowledged
before. The trick, of course, is to see all risks for what they are before
the fact. Indeed, they must be seen before the fact if there is to be any
attempt to quantify them. Unfortunately, when so many risks formerly seen as
incredible become credible, we face the danger of sensory and numerical
overload.
In the coming months and years, questions of risk will continue to arise.
As with most such questions in the past, the answers will probably depend
upon the interpretation of numbers rather than on the numbers themselves.
Ordering risks will ultimately come down to political and emotional
decisions. It is imperative that, no matter what their math skills, our
elected leaders and decision makers are well prepared to deal with those
components of risk assessment.
Henry Petroski is the A. S. Vesic Professor of Civil Engineering and a
professor of history at Duke University. His latest book is "Paperboy:
Confessions of a Future Engineer" (Knopf).
************************************************************************
You are currently subscribed to the Radsafe mailing list. To unsubscribe,
send an e-mail to Majordomo@list.vanderbilt.edu Put the text "unsubscribe
radsafe" (no quote marks) in the body of the e-mail, with no subject line.
You can view the Radsafe archives at http://www.vanderbilt.edu/radsafe/